Understanding decisions: The power of combining psychology and economics

Categories
Centre for Decision Research

Shilo Rea is Director of Media Relations at Carnegie Mellon University and represents Dietrich College of Humanities & Social Sciences, including the areas of psychology, decision sciences, education and literature. Shilo wrote the original article, interviewing Professors Wändi Bruine de Bruin and Baruch Fischhoff about their research, which this blog post is based on. Wändi Bruine de Bruin is Professor with University Leadership Chair in Behavioural Decision Making at Leeds University Business School and Co-Director of the Centre for Decision Research. Baruch Fischhoff is the Howard Heinz University Professor in the Institute for Politics and Strategy and Department of Engineering and Public Policy at Carnegie Mellon University and member of the Centre for Decision Research. The research discussed in this blog post was supported by grants from the U.S. National Science Foundation, Swedish Foundation for the Humanities and Social Sciences and the European Union Seventh Framework Program.

Wandi Bruine de Bruin and Baruch Fischhoff.

Adolescents face many challenging decisions. So do consumers.

Psychology and economics are both interested in how people make decisions, but have different theories and methods. In our work with economists at Northwestern, Michigan, the Federal Reserve and elsewhere, we have found ways to complement each other’s expertise.

In two series of studies that focused on individuals’ expectations for major life events, we worked with economists to design survey questions that were simple enough for laypeople to answer but precise enough to inform economic models.

The first project examined adolescents’ expectations for life events that would affect their psychological and economic development, such as finding work, being arrested and having children. Our colleagues in economics, led by Charles Manski, a former CMU faculty member, wanted to ask precise questions on a major national survey but were meeting resistance from survey researchers, who claimed that they were too hard for teenagers to answer.

We supported the economists’ concerns with studies showing that questions using seemingly simpler language were actually more difficult for respondents and less useful for researchers, because the simpler wording was more ambiguous. We then developed questions that teenagers could understand and provide answers that economists could use. The process included asking for numerical probabilities (e.g. 70 percent), rather than verbal quantifiers, such as "very likely."

We found that youngsters were better at judging their futures than people may have thought, that they could estimate with numerical probabilities just fine and their answers were generally sensible.

The second project involved consumers’ expectations of inflation, which play a central role in predicting financial decisions for the overall economy. Economists at the U.S. Federal Reserve worried that the questions that they had used for decades did not mean the same thing to consumers as they did for economists.

Bringing psychological methods to bear on economics problems, we found that here, too, it was better to ask more precise questions. When people were asked directly about 'inflation,' it led to less confusion, and more accurate expectations, than when vague terms were used, like 'prices in general’.

Moreover, asking about 'prices in general' led people to think of prices for specific goods, bringing more extreme prices to mind. As a result, expectations for ‘prices in general’ were higher than expectations for 'inflation.'

There are four conditions that made such transdiciplinary research possible: having a shared research goal, which neither discipline could achieve on its own; finding common ground in shared methodology; sharing effort throughout, with common language and sense of ownership; and gaining mutual benefit from both the research process and its products.

Successful collaborations across fields can be done. You need to find willing partners, and create trusted partnerships. For people interested in decision research, you should look for work that combines both psychology and economics because neither can provide the complete picture.

You can read the published paper on this research in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

The original news article this blog post is based on was first published on the Carnegie Mellon University website. 

Contact us

If you would like to get in touch regarding any of these blog entries, or are interested in contributing to the blog, please contact:

Email: research.lubs@leeds.ac.uk
Phone: +44 (0)113 343 8754

Click here to view our privacy statement. You can repost this blog article, following the terms listed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International licence.

The views expressed in this article are those of the author and may not reflect the views of Leeds University Business School or the University of Leeds.